Radboud Annals of Medical Students

RAMS

  • Home
  • News
    • Newsletter archive
  • About
    • RAMS
    • The Board
      • The General Board
        • The General Board (2021-2022)
        • Previous General Boards
      • The Editorial Board
        • The Editorial Board (2021-2022)
        • Previous Editorial Boards
    • The Supervisory Board
    • Reviewers and Editors
    • RAMS Committees
      • Symposium Committee
      • Masterclass Committee
  • Editions
    • 2021-2022
      • Twentieth Edition – September 2021
      • Twentyfirst Edition – December 2021
      • Twenty second Edition – March 2022
    • 2020-2021
      • Sixteenth edition – September 2020
      • Seventeenth edition – December 2020
      • Eighteenth edition – March 2021
      • Nineteenth Edition – June 2021
    • 2019-2020
      • Fourteenth edition – December 2019
      • Fifteenth edition – April 2020
      • Sixteenth edition – September 2020
    • 2018-2019
      • Eleventh Edition – September 2018
      • Twelfth Edition – January 2019
      • Thirteenth edition – May 2019
    • 2017 – 2018
      • Ninth Edition – November 2017
      • Tenth Edition – February 2018
    • 2016-2017
      • Sixth Edition – November 2016
      • Seventh Edition – March 2017
      • Eighth Edition – July 2017
    • 2015-2016
      • Third Edition – November 2015
      • Fourth Edition – March 2016
      • Fifth Edition – June 2016
    • 2014-2015
      • Pilot Edition – June 2014
      • First Edition – January 2015
      • Second Edition – June 2015
  • For Authors
    • Submit your Article
    • Research internship
  • For Supervisors
  • For Reviewers
  • Contact
  • Privacy policy
Je bent hier: Home / News / To See or Not to See

To See or Not to See

4 april 2021 by Rams

About Anton-Babinski Syndrome and Blindsight

Lessa Schippers

Would you rather be blind and be under the impression that you can see, or think you are blind, while you can actually see? The former is called Anton syndrome, and the latter blindsight. This brief message will discuss both of these rare conditions involving vision and vision loss. 

Anton-Babinski syndrome is named after the neurologist Gabriel Anton, who defined the disease at the end of the 19th century; however, the syndrome was already described in 63 AD. by Seneca [1,2]. Patients with Anton syndrome suffer from cortical blindness due to damage in the occipital lobe. This syndrome is often caused by stroke, but other causes can include head trauma or hypertensive encephalopathy [1]. Interestingly, these patients are unable to recognise the fact that they are visually impaired. In general, a condition related to the inability of recognising an impairment is called anosognosia, a term coined by François Babinski in 1914 [3]. In the case of Anton-Babinski syndrome, other brain areas besides the occipital lobe, such as speech-language areas, construct made-up images due to a disconnection between brain areas [3]. This construction of images is called confabulation [3]. Due to these images, patients believe they are still able to see. For example, if you ask a patient with Anton-Babinski syndrome to comment on your outfit, and you wear a dress, they might respond, ‘I really like your pants’. Confabulation can also occur in the case of deafness or amputations. Treatment consists of treating the origin of the occipital damage to the extent possible [1].

On the other hand, blindsight is a condition where patients think that they cannot see, although these patients should be able to see. However, unconsciously they do see. For instance, if you ask them to describe an object you are presenting to them, e.g., a red mug, they will tell you that they are blind and cannot see the object. If you ask them to guess the object, they will most likely guess, unconsciously, right. Blindsight is caused by damage in the V1 area, the primary visual cortex, in the brain [4]. Patients with blindsight can still see because the V1 area is not the only area in the brain receiving input from the visual tract [4]. Some other areas receiving input from the visual tract are extrastriate visual areas, the inferotemporal cortex, and the amygdala [4]. In this way, patients unconsciously receive visual information, but they lack coordination by the V1-area to put the pieces together, creating the final image we consciously see. Treatment can consist of either interventions that try to recover vision or learning to use aids for people who are blind[5]. 

We presented two different syndromes here, that can be seen as polar opposites regarding their symptomology. Despite this remarkable disparity, the pathophysiology is curiously similar. It is only the location of the brain damage that decides which syndrome a patient will develop. This delicateness of the brain fascinates and drives neuroscientists every day.

References:

[1] J, M.D. & Naqvi, I.A. Anton Syndrome. in StatPearls, StatPearls Publishing LLC., Treasure Island (FL), 2021).

[2] André, C. Seneca and the First Description of Anton Syndrome. J Neuroophthalmol 38, 511-513 (2018).

[3] Maddula, M., et al. Anton’s syndrome due to cerebrovascular disease: a case report. J Med Case Rep 3, 9028-9028 (2009).

[4] Ajina, S. & Bridge, H. Blindsight and Unconscious Vision: What They Teach Us about the Human Visual System. Neuroscientist 23, 529-541 (2016).

[5] Das, A., et al. Beyond blindsight: properties of visual relearning in cortically blind fields. J Neurosci 34, 11652-11664 (2014).

Categorie: News, Uncategorized

RAMS Newsletter

RAMS complies to the GDPR. By submitting your e-mail address to us you agree with our Privacy Policy.

Social Media


Sponsors & Partners

  • Brian Gardner
  • Lauren Mancke
  • Nathan Rice
  • Nick Croft
  • Rafal Tomal
  • Ron Rennick
© Copyright 2014 RADBOUD ANNALS OF MEDICAL STUDENTS · All Rights Reserved · Powered by PLatform11
Beheer cookie toestemming

Om de beste ervaringen te bieden, gebruiken wij technologieën zoals cookies om informatie over je apparaat op te slaan en/of te raadplegen. Door in te stemmen met deze technologieën kunnen wij gegevens zoals surfgedrag of unieke ID's op deze site verwerken. Als je geen toestemming geeft of uw toestemming intrekt, kan dit een nadelige invloed hebben op bepaalde functies en mogelijkheden.

Functioneel Altijd actief
De technische opslag of toegang is strikt noodzakelijk voor het legitieme doel het gebruik mogelijk te maken van een specifieke dienst waarom de abonnee of gebruiker uitdrukkelijk heeft gevraagd, of met als enig doel de uitvoering van de transmissie van een communicatie over een elektronisch communicatienetwerk.
Voorkeuren
De technische opslag of toegang is noodzakelijk voor het legitieme doel voorkeuren op te slaan die niet door de abonnee of gebruiker zijn aangevraagd.
Statistieken
De technische opslag of toegang die uitsluitend voor statistische doeleinden wordt gebruikt. De technische opslag of toegang die uitsluitend wordt gebruikt voor anonieme statistische doeleinden. Zonder dagvaarding, vrijwillige naleving door uw Internet Service Provider, of aanvullende gegevens van een derde partij, kan informatie die alleen voor dit doel wordt opgeslagen of opgehaald gewoonlijk niet worden gebruikt om je te identificeren.
Marketing
De technische opslag of toegang is nodig om gebruikersprofielen op te stellen voor het verzenden van reclame, of om de gebruiker op een website of over verschillende websites te volgen voor soortgelijke marketingdoeleinden.
Beheer opties Beheer diensten Beheer leveranciers Lees meer over deze doeleinden
Bekijk voorkeuren
{title} {title} {title}